Skip to main content
Erschienen in:
Buchtitelbild

Open Access 2022 | OriginalPaper | Buchkapitel

Energy Prosumers’ Spillovers and the Policy Effect: Comparing Two Alpine Valleys in Styria and South Tyrol

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

The article identifies and compares spillover effects of prosumers (electricity producers and consumers) in different incentive regimes. Prosumers are expected to reduce their energy consumption and increase their environmentally friendly behavior. Such spillover effects are promising for sustainability initiatives – however, researchers have not demonstrated these behavioral effects consistently. I hypothesize that these inconsistent results are related to how households were induced to become prosumers in the first place. To test this hypothesis, prosumers and non-prosumers of different incentive regimes are compared. Statistics show that monetary incentives can lead to adverse spillover effects and that collective prosumers generate the strongest positive spillovers. Results encourage sustainability initiatives to offset financial benefits to promote sustainable lifestyles.

1 Energy Behavior and Its Spillovers

Almost all human activities are directly (electricity, heat, mobility, etc.) or indirectly (food, clothing, etc.) linked to energy consumption (Pothitou et al. 2016). Thus, to overcome the energy and climate crises, we need a new energy lifestyle, not just technical solutions to reduce the dependence on fossil fuels. Therefore, technical solutions (e.g., reducing CO2 emissions per unit of energy produced, increasing energy efficiency) are as important as changing human energy behavior (Rosenbloom 2017; Steg et al. 2021). However, unlike technical solutions, it is more difficult to change energy behavior as it comprises a broad variety of patterns related to the consumption or generation of energy. These are ranging from everyday routines (e.g., turning on the lights) that require little cognitive effort as they are mainly shaped by habits and social practices, to one-shot behaviors (e.g., installing a new energy system) that require more cognitive effort because they involve a conscious decision-making process (Dütschke et al. 2021). In this respect, people’s everyday energy behaviors occur mostly unconsciously, in the context of repetitive, habitual behaviors, or indirectly through other behaviors (Axon et al. 2018; Verplanken and Roy 2016). Consequently, people are usually not even aware of how much energy they consume (Pothitou et al. 2016). As the interaction between technologies, practices, and norms locks people in “old” behavior patterns (Axon et al. 2018; Maréchal 2010), sustainability initiatives face the challenge of having to change habitual behavior (Axon et al. 2018). And that is the crux: Changing habits is not easy (Moberg et al. 2019). Researchers recommend focusing on the context in which habitual behaviors occur – if the context changes, new behaviors are more likely to be established (Baum and Gross 2017; Maréchal 2010; Miller et al. 2015; Verplanken and Roy 2016).
In this regard, photovoltaic (PV) systems are promising as they turn electricity consumers into so-called prosumers (producers and consumers of electricity combined in one person), and thus, completely change people’s energy context (Kotilainen and Saari 2018; Palm et al. 2018). For prosumers, electricity becomes more tangible and touchable as a commodity, in contrast to consumers, who are usually unaware of where and how the electricity they consume is produced (Braito et al. 2017). In addition, prosumers typically receive feedback from the electricity-generating entity (Dütschke et al. 2021). This allows prosumers to observe, reflect, and reconsider their energy lifestyles (Stedmon et al. 2013).
For understanding changes in energy lifestyle, the “material participation” theory (Marres 2012) has its merits. It is an object-oriented perspective and assumes that using a novel energy technology can lead to new energy practices. For example, when PV panels become objects of participation and engagement, they can promote material participation as energy citizens (Ryghaug et al. 2018). This shift from a passive, habitual energy consumer to an active energy citizen is a significant change in people’s energy reality (Palm et al. 2018) that can trigger additional behavioral effects, so-called spillovers1, which can lead to more or less compliance in the transition to a more sustainable (energy) lifestyle. Positive spillover effects on the environment can support the energy transition (Carrico 2021).
From a scientific perspective, spillovers are interesting because they make lifestyle change observable by directly focusing on holistic relationships between behavioral change within and across contexts (Galizzi and Whitmarsh 2019). Prosumers may experience energy-related and non-energy-related spillover effects.
Energy-related spillovers.
Energy becomes a tangible commodity for prosumers, no longer invisible and always available. When prosumers generate renewable electricity and adjust or reduce their electricity consumption accordingly – e.g., by doing the laundry when the sun is shining (Braito et al. 2017) – the sustainability effect doubles. As a result, prosumers generate double dividend effects, which can lead to a 6 % reduction in total electricity consumption, according to Keirstead (2007). However, the spillover effect is negative when prosumers increase their electricity consumption. In the literature, this is explained by the rebound effect, a psychological phenomenon rooted in moral licensing (Dütschke et al. 2021; Seebauer 2018; Sorrell et al. 2020). By generating “green”, low-carbon electricity, prosumers give themselves the “license” to consume more energy.
Non-energy-related spillovers.
When prosumers start to behave environmentally conscious in other areas, for example, by saving water, avoiding waste, or changing their mobility behavior, positive spillovers arise (Dolan and Galizzi 2015; Ryghaug et al. 2018; Truelove et al. 2014). This is mainly attributed to changing attitudes (Henn et al. 2020) or a new ecological self-image of prosumers (Sloot et al. 2018). There seems to be a more holistic change in attitudes among prosumers, which is a prerequisite for establishing a new lifestyle (Carrico 2021). However, rebound effects can also occur in non-energy-related behaviors. For example, prosumers indicate that they feel their air travels are justified, because they have already contributed to sustainable energy production (Braito et al. 2017).

2 Ambiguous Empirical Findings on Spillovers and the Attempt to Elucidate Them

In the literature, spillovers are discussed ambiguously – some studies find positive, others adverse, and even others no such effects at all (Dütschke et al. 2021; Geiger et al. 2021; Nash et al. 2017; Sintov et al. 2019; Stikvoort et al. 2020; Truelove et al. 2014). For instance, Palm et al. (2018) failed to demonstrate that prosumers in Sweden changed their energy consumption to a reasonable extent. Sorrell et al. (2020) found rebound effects, which erode energy and emission savings. Steinhorst et al. (2015) analyzed the energy-saving behavior of German consumers and found no spillover effects when the behavior change was economically framed. Similarly, Dütschke et al. (2021) studied Swedish prosumers and concluded that spillovers depend on the design of financial incentives. According to Maki et al. (2019), the ambiguous empirical findings on spillovers are due to the fact that different studies measure miscellaneous things: actual behavior, behavioral intentions, self-reported behavior, or the support for policies. Instead, Truelove et al. (2014) assert that studies are disjointed and come from different disciplines, and Palm et al. (2018) consider the numbers of studies that measure prosumers’ electricity consumption to be too small.
In this article, I present and test an alternative explanation. The underlying idea stems from a previous study (Braito et al. 2017), in which we found that energy policies have not only primary effects on encouraging PV investments but also secondary ones, such as crowding-in/-out segments of a society. We found that generous monetary incentives attract (crowding-in) households that pursue PV investments for economic considerations and an anthropocentric relationship with nature but tend to discourage (crowding-out) those groups that embrace non-monetary considerations as well as the motivations for collective energy projects.
The research question I now face is whether the ambiguous picture of spillovers may also be related to how households were induced to become prosumers in the first place. In other words: How do prosumers’ spillovers differ across policy regimes? The hypothesis is that higher financial incentives reduce spillovers. The idea that policies have not only the primary (intended) effect but also secondary effects is not entirely new. Baum and Gross (2017) classified secondary behavioral effects of policies and developed a behavior change framework that explicitly incorporates secondary behavioral effects. However, prosumers from different policy contexts have not yet been compared to test the effects of policy measures. In this regard, the article aims to examine descriptively and comparatively whether the hypothesis that prosumers in varying incentive schemes exhibit different spillover effects is valid.
Contributing to the sparse literature on spillover effects of prosumers, this article offers two novel contributions. First, the analysis assesses not only electricity-related behavior of prosumers, but also behaviors from other areas. So far, behavioral studies of prosumers focused mainly on electricity consumption patterns (Maki et al. 2019). Broadening the research perspective to include spillovers provides a better understanding of how successful behavior change intervention might be to disrupt habitual behaviors and stimulate more sustainable lifestyles. Second, the analysis explores the role of policies in disentangling the mixed and confusing results that research on prosumer spillover effects produced so far. A better understanding of what strengthens or weakens spillovers is valuable for the transition literature and can be helpful to practitioners considering behavior change interventions.

3 Methodology and Research Design

The analysis presented here is based on a data set from a previous study (Braito et al. 2017). It consists of a large-scale questionnaire survey of prosumers and non-prosumers conducted in two Alpine regions in 2015: i) Puster valley (Pustertal) in the province of South Tyrol, (Italy, IT), and ii) Mur valley (Murtal) in the province of Styria (Austria, AT). The two neighboring countries pursued different strategies to stimulate private investment in PV. Italy supported private investments with generous financial subsidies until 2015 (Orioli and Di Gangi 2016). Austria offered much lower financial support, but emphasized the societal and ecological added value of energy production through PV (Braito et al. 2017). Besides private investment in PV, this also encouraged the establishment of citizen solar power plants (CSPP), which allow investment costs to be shared and are seen “… as a form of empowerment, giving bottom-up initiatives control over energy infrastructures and channeling revenues to local communities” (Schreuer 2016, p. 126). Apart from the differences in PV policies, the two study regions are very similar geographically, culturally, historically, and socioeconomically. Typically, it is difficult for behavioral science disciplines to conduct comparative cross-national studies because too many contextual factors interact with the research objectives. However, the cultural-historical identity of South Tyrol2 and the Austrian Alpine region is comparable. So, it is possible to conduct a “quasi” ex-post field experiment3, where the two different PV policies are the experimental intervention.
The Mur valley was selected for comparison with the Puster valley in an iterative exclusion procedure. The questionnaire survey was conducted between October 2014 and March 2015. The language of the questionnaire was German, and only German-speaking respondents in South Tyrol were interviewed to ensure comparability between the two subsamples and to control for cultural heterogeneity (for a detailed description of the selection of case studies and recruitment of participants, see Braito et al. 2017).
The sampling design distinguishes five respondent groups (see Fig. 1). In both regions, individual prosumers (PVAT, PVIT) and non-prosumers (nPVAT, nPVIT) were surveyed. Collective prosumers involved in citizen solar power plants (CSPP) could only be studied in Austria, as such collective PV initiatives had only just become established in Austria at the time of data collection (Braito 2017). This group was surveyed to understand whether being an individual or collective prosumer triggers comparable spillovers. The two groups of non-prosumers represent the control groups of the quasi-experimental research design. Spillover effects can be identified by comparing prosumers and non-prosumers within the same country. In addition, by comparing the two control groups across countries allows to examine which behavioral elements are admissible. Although, the two regions are very similar, it must be assumed that citizens from different countries do not have quite the same behavioral options, whether for infrastructural reasons (e.g., no public transportation network, no renewable energy supply) or simply because of different social practices. Thus, if the cross-country comparison of non-prosumers showed significant differences, it is assumed that the behavior of society in general is not comparable, and therefore the comparison of prosumers is also not valid.

3.1 Questionnaire and Data Collection

A behavioral scale with nineteen items was used for the survey to measure the self-reported energy-related and non-energy-related everyday behaviors of prosumers in the private sector4. Items derived and adapted from previous research (Braito 2017; Gifford et al. 2011; Mirosa et al. 2013) were cross-checked with regional information campaigns (BMK 2013; Statistik Austria 2009) to either adapt them to regional contexts or exclude those that were not appropriate for the case study context. Respondents were asked to self-report their daily behavior on a five-point rating scale ranging from “1 = never” to “5 = always”.
Ten survey items were used to measure participants’ energy-related behaviors. They captured energy conservation and the extent to which respondents are concerned with energy production and consumption, i.e., their energy awareness, which inevitably leads to increased environmental behaviors (Nash et al. 2019). Nine survey items were used to measure participants’ non-energy-related behaviors. The items cover food, mobility, and waste, and thus capture environmental self-identity, which can contribute to sustainable lifestyles (Lacasse 2016; Lanzini and Thøgersen 2014; Sloot et al. 2018) (see Table 1).
Table 1
Survey items. Own illustration
Category
Items in the questionnaire
Labels in the analysis
Energy-related behavior
Energy saving
I turn the lights off when I leave a room.
Lights off
I use energy-efficient devices.
Efficient devices
I turn off my electronic devices when I do not need them and ensure that they are not in “stand-by mode”.
Devices off
I (don’t) dry my clothes in a tumble dryer.a
No tumble dryer
I use energy-saving light sources.
Energy-saving bulb
I obtain green electricity.
Green electricity
Energy awareness
I check my electricity meter.
Check electricity meter
When using electricity, I reflect where it comes from.
Reflect electricity source
I consciously consider the energy consumption when heating or cooling my apartment/house.
Control energy consumption
In our family, we discuss environmental and energy related topics.
Discuss environmental topics
Non-energy-related behavior
Food
I buy environmentally friendly products.
Buy environmentally friendly
I buy organic food.
Buy organic
I buy regional and seasonal food.
Buy regional
I ensure to eat meat rarely.
Reduce meat consumption
Mobility
As an alternative to the car, I use public transport, the bike, or I walk.
Reduce car use
I (don’t) take the airplane to reach my holiday destination.a
Reduce air travel
Waste
I (don’t) like to buy new clothes.a
Reduce cloth consumption
I ensure to produce little garbage.
Reduce waste production
I sort my garbage.
Sort garbage
aThis item was phrased negatively in the questionnaire to check the reliability of respondents’ answers. The responses were inversely coded for statistical analysis
Data were collected using a “drop-off/pick-up method” (Allred and Ross-Davis 2011; Steele et al. 2001) and by postal mail. Since PV installations are visible, I visited households that had or had not invested in PV, 150 per group, i.e., 300 in each valley. I distributed the questionnaires, collected the completed ones at an agreed date, or asked the participants to send back the completed questionnaire in a return envelope. This procedure allowed to systematically randomize the participants in the control group (nPV) by only interviewing nPV respondents, who were about five houses away from the PV respondents interviewed. This ensured that different contexts (e.g., another village with a different socioeconomic structure) and proximity to a house with PV did not bias the representativeness of this group. The fifth group, consisting of collective PV investors, is not visible and could only be addressed through a postal survey. The company organizing CSPP in Styria – Unser Kraftwerk (2012) – sent 600 questionnaires and prepaid return envelopes to households participating in such collectively financed PV systems. After cleaning the data set for missing records5, the final data set consists of 580 questionnaires, which corresponds to an acceptable response rate: 58–83 % for the drop-off/pick-up method and 26 % for postal mail (for details on response rate and sample description see Braito 2017).

3.2 Statistical Analysis

The questionnaires were manually transferred to Excel and then imported into SPSS. First, data quality was checked (outlier flagging rule with a g-value of 2.2), and data were described univariately. Prosumers’ self-reported behaviors measured on a five-point rating scale is treated as continuous data as it consists of more than one item, and the intervals between response options are equal (Norman 2010). Since the standard deviations in the compared subsamples are about the same, the different sample sizes of the five groups do not pose a statistical issue. The internal reliability of the overall behavior scales is good, with a Cronbach’s alpha of .77.
I then calculated bivariate mean comparisons to identify spillover effects – i.e., causal relationships cannot be derived. The goal was to examine whether the behaviors of prosumers and non-prosumers differ comparatively. Therefore, I first examined the behaviors in the respective study regions (vertical axis in Fig. 1) and compared the behaviors of prosumers with that of non-prosumers (PVIT vs. nPVIT, PVAT vs. nPVAT, CSPP vs. nPVAT). I then examined the spillover effects themselves to determine the extent to which the behaviors of the three prosumer groups could be compared (PVAT vs. CSPP, PVAT vs. PVIT). The goal was to determine whether spillover effects differ across incentive regimes (horizontal axis in Fig. 1). For the cross-country comparison, it was first necessary to compare the control groups (nPVAT vs. nPVIT) to identify all items that do not allow for a cross-country comparison. In case of statistically relevant differences, I suspended the comparison of the behaviors of prosumers from the Mur valley and the Puster valley.
Since the data are not normally distributed6, I used the non-parametric alternative to the independent t-test, the Mann–Whitney U test. This test compares a difference in the dependent variable for two independent groups when the dependent variable is either ordinal or continuous but not normally distributed. The test ranks all dependent values and then uses the sum of the ranks for each group to calculate the test statistic (for a detailed description of the test, see Field et al. 2012).
I used SPSS’s new procedure because it simultaneously generates the population pyramid, which is necessary to test whether the distributions of the two groups of independent variables are similarly distributed. Apart from this, SPSS outputs several values, including the U-statistic and the z-scores. The U-value is the number of cross-sample pairs where an observation from the first sample exceeds an observation from the second sample. The z-score is compared with the standard normal quantiles to obtain the significance value. If z is less than −1.96 or greater than 1.96, the test is significant at p < .05, and thus, the null hypothesis, i.e., no statistically significant difference between the compared groups, must be rejected. In this case, I calculated the effect size. This is done by dividing the absolute z-score by the square root of the number of pairs. According to Cohen’s classification of effect sizes (r), a value of r ≥ .1 is considered a small effect, r ≥ .3 is considered a moderate effect, and r ≥ .5 is considered a large effect. A value of r = .1 implies that about 10 % of the variability can be attributed to the independent variables of group affiliation.

4 Identifying and Reflecting Prosumer Spillovers

When comparing the behavior of prosumers and non-prosumers, some spillover effects stand out. Table 2 provides the test statistics where the group comparison between prosumers and the control group of non-prosumers shows statistically significant differences. The effect size r of the differences is less than .3 (i.e., relatively small), but this does not detract from significance. As mentioned in sec. 3.2, a value of .2, for example, means that 20 % of the difference is due to the independent variable (belonging to a group). The results reflect previous studies: The spillover effects of prosumers are likely to be relatively small (Palm et al. 2018), negligible (Geiger et al. 2021), or even negative (Dütschke et al. 2021).
Table 2
Statistically significant differences between prosumer and non-prosumer behaviors. Own illustration
Label
Prosumers vs. non-prosumers
PVIT vs. nPVIT
PVAT vs. nPVAT
CSPP vs. nPVAT
U,z
r
U,z
r
U,z
r
Energy-related behavior
Devices off
    
7173, −3.73***
 + .22
No tumble dryer
4285, −2.93**
−.20
  
7402, −3.32***
 + .20
Energy-saving bulb
  
4425, −2.03*
 + .14
  
Check electricity meter
3999, −3.09**
 + .22
3645, −3.84***
 + .27
  
Reflect electricity source
  
3998, −2.88**
 + .20
7791, −2.34*
 + .14
Control energy consumption
    
8094, −2.58**
 + .15
Discuss environmental topics
    
7098, −3.60***
 + .22
Non-energy-related behavior
Buy regional
  
4204, −2.57*
 + .18
  
Reduce meat consumption
    
7896, −2.64**
 + .16
Reduce car use
4129, −2.75**
−.19
  
7707, −2.88**
 + .17
Reduce air travel
    
7965, −2.29*
−.14
Reduce cloth consumption
4573, −2.32*
 + .16
  
7587, −2.93***
 + .18
A description of the test statistics is given in sec. 3.2: U is the Mann–Whitney U test score, z is used to obtain the p-value (* p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001) and r is the effect size (the plus/minus in front of r indicates if the “first” or “second” group of the comparison performs the respective behavior more (+) or less (−) often).
PVAT/PVIT … Individual prosumers of photovoltaic solar energy in the Mur valley, Austria/the Puster valley, Italy; nPVAT/nPVIT … Individual non-prosumers of photovoltaic solar energy in the Mur valley, Austria/the Puster valley, Italy; CSPP … Collective prosumers involved in citizen solar power plants in Austria

4.1 Individual Prosumer Spillovers in the Puster Valley

For PVIT, a mixed picture emerges, as both positive and negative spillovers can be observed here (see Table 2). Thus, the behavioral effects are rather contradictory than coherent. Conversely, PVIT check their electricity meter more regularly and buy new clothes less often than nPVIT. Hence, PVIT use the tumble dryer and the car more often than nPVIT. These two behaviors are energy-intensive behaviors (Nash et al. 2017), and thus, the results here might point to the rebound effect. For rebound effects to occur, prosumers must be aware of the impact of their behavior on the environment (Truelove et al. 2014) or the energy costs (Freeman 2018). This seems to be the case for PVIT as they reportedly care more about government subsidies than the environmental impact of the PV system (Braito et al. 2017). The finding that PVIT check the electricity meter more often than nPVIT can be considered an economic motivation. Namely, they see how much electricity, and thus money, they save. In summary, PVIT are unlikely to be more environmentally friendly  than nPVIT, and they generate negative rather than positive spillovers. Generous financial incentives seem unlikely to motivate anyone to do more than the incentive intended (Henn et al. 2020), thus, limiting secondary or additional behavioral effects (Maki et al. 2019).

4.2 Individual Prosumer Spillovers in the Mur Valley

No adverse or contradictory behavioral effects can be identified among PVAT. They use energy-saving light bulbs and check their electricity meters more often, pay more attention to the origin of electricity, and reach for regional and seasonal food more often than nPVAT. However, all these differences are “simple” behaviors that require little effort – after all, they fall into the category of energy awareness and support for regional value creation. Therefore, the ecological impact of the behavioral differences is likely to be relatively small compared to nPVAT, and rather on the level of higher sustainability awareness. This is in line with the results of Wittenberg and Matthies (2016, p. 209), who show for Germany “… that households with a PV system do not report lower electricity consumption but higher environmental motivation …”.
Regarding non-energy-related behavior, the results show that PVAT are more likely to buy regional products and seem to be more aware of the value of regional value creation. This could be because they feel part of the regional energy production or that the awareness of regional value creation was one of the reasons why this group invested in a PV system in the first place. Alternatively, Braito et al. (2017) noted that PVAT had high levels of intrinsic motivation, ecological values, and anthropocentric human-nature relationships. Either way, sustainability awareness has the catalytic potential for further spillover effects (Nash et al. 2019; Nilsson et al. 2017), and the emergence of new lifestyles (Pothitou et al. 2016).

4.3 Collective Prosumer Spillovers in the Mur Valley

The results for CSPP show the largest spillover effects. They seem to achieve a double dividend with their energy-related behaviors. Compared to nPVAT, they are more likely to report engaging in energy-saving behaviors, such as turning off devices instead of putting them on standby, or not using the tumble dryer. Compared to nPVAT, CSPP are also more likely to agonize about the origin of electricity, the energy used for heating or cooling, and are more likely to discuss energy-related issues as a family. This group also shows more non-energy-related spillover effects than PVAT. For example, CSPP are more likely to report eating meat less often, replacing their car with an alternative, or buying new clothes less often. Hence, CSPP seem to take airplanes more often to get to the vacation destination.
There are several rationales why CSPP maintain a more sustainable lifestyle than nPVAT. First, CSPP are the oldest with the highest education (Braito et al. 2017). Education, in particular, may foster a stronger commitment to sustainability (Powdthavee 2021). Second, it may be that CSPP generally feel a high level of social and environmental responsibility. Although the financial cost of a collective PV system is not comparable to that of a private PV system, the financial return is also not as high. In this respect, the motivation would have to be altruistic rather than financial (Braito et al. 2017). And third, it could be that participation in a collective project leads to increased awareness of collective goods (Barth 2021). Either way, the results are exciting and encourage further consideration of how motivation to participate in collective goods might help promote or reinforce sustainable lifestyles.

4.4 Prosumer Spillovers in Different Policy Regimes

Comparing the behavior of the three prosumer groups (PVAT, CSPP, PVIT) shows to what extent the spillover effects differ under the various incentive regimes. Table 3 shows the test statistics, again only for those cases where statistically significant differences are evident. It should be noted that the effect sizes are relatively small (r is always less than .3). However, these do not affect the results, as it is noteworthy that statistically relevant differences are discernible.
Table 3
Significant differences between prosumer behaviors that belong to different PV incentive programs. Own illustration
 
Individual vs. collective framing
Low vs. high monetary framing
Label
PVAT vs. CSPP
PVAT vs. PVIT
 
U,z
r
U,z
r
Energy-related behavior
Efficient devices
  
Comparison suspended
Devices off
  
3314, −2.80
 + .21
No tumble dryer
4913, −3.10**
−.20
Comparison suspended
Energy-saving bulb
5436, −2.64**
 + .17
3478, −2.05
 + .15
Green electricity
  
Comparison suspended
Check electricity meter
5089, −3.19**
 + .20
3487, −2.08
 + .15
Reflect electricity source
  
3427, −2.12
 + .16
Non-energy-related behavior
Buy environmentally friendly
  
3091, −3.26
 + .24
Buy organic
  
Comparison suspended
Buy regional
  
3441, −2.18
 + .16
Reduce meat consumption
5420, −2.30*
−.15
  
Reduce car use
5488, −2.21*
−.14
Comparison suspended
Reduce air travel
5047, −3.12**
 + .20
  
Sort garbage
  
3854, −2.02
 + .15
A description of the test statistics is given in sec. 3.2: U is the Mann–Whitney U test score, z is used to obtain the p value (* p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001) and r is the effect size (the plus/minus in front of r indicates if the “first” or “second” group of the comparison performs the respective behavior more (+) or less (−) often).
PVAT/PVIT … Individual prosumers of photovoltaic solar energy in the Mur valley, Austria/the Puster valley, Italy; nPVAT/nPVIT … Individual non-prosumers of photovoltaic solar energy in the Mur valley, Austria/the Puster valley, Italy; CSPP … Collective prosumers involved in citizen solar power plants in Austria; Comparison suspended … A significant difference of the control groups (nPVIT, nPVAT) indicates that regarding this item the behaviour PVAT and PVIT in general is not comparable.
Table 3 (column “individual vs. collective framing”) shows how the behaviors of PVAT and CSPP differ. This is to get to the bottom of the question whether individuals who own a PV system behave more or less environmentally friendly than those who have financially participated in a collective PV system. This comparison does not provide a clear picture. Both groups show behaviors that generate positive spillover effects – but in different areas. PVAT use more energy-saving light sources and check their electricity meter more often, while CSPP use the clothes dryer less often. Results are similarly mixed for non-energy-related behaviors. CSPP eat meat less often and use alternatives to the car more often. However, they also use airplanes more often to reach their vacation destination.
Table 3 (column “low vs. high monetary framing”) compares PVAT and PVIT. This is to get to the bottom of the question whether more or less environmentally friendly behavior and thus spillovers are stimulated when individuals are motivated to invest in a PV system by high or moderate financial incentives. This comparison shows a clear picture. PVAT outperform PVIT in four energy-related and three non-energy-related behaviors. PVAT achieve more double-dividend effects by switching off appliances more often instead of putting them in standby mode, using energy-saving light bulbs, checking the electricity meter, and being more concerned about the origin of electricity. It seems that PVAT adjust their energy consumption much more than PVIT and also have a higher electricity awareness. Furthermore, PVAT are more likely to buy environmentally friendly products as well as regional and seasonal food, and separate waste more often. Thus, higher monetary incentives in South Tyrol do not lead to more environmentally friendly behavior. It could be countered that this is due to the fact that people in these countries generally have different behaviors. However, as the comparison of the control groups shows, the behavior of the citizens of the two countries is comparable in these respects and was not excluded from the analysis.
In summary, the comparison of PVAT and PVIT suggests, that energy habits can be changed by investing in PV systems, but that the motivation for this engagement matters if a new energy lifestyle develops as a result. Because the spillover effects in the Mur valley are positive and more substantial, although the control groups are not statistically significantly different, the study shows that policy may have been strong enough to influence the magnitude of the behavioral effects.

5 Discussion and Conclusions

This research sought to enhance our understanding of prosumer spillover effects and thus contribute to the ambiguous literature. The results indicate that overly generous financial incentives not only carry the hidden costs of crowding-out intrinsic motivation (Braito et al. 2017), but also increase the likelihood of adverse spillover effects. Although market-based strategies can effectively promote environmentally friendly behavior (Maki et al. 2016) because they increase (economic) self-interest in, for example, energy-saving behavior (Pothitou et al. 2016), questions are also repeatedly raised about whether they support stable and sustainable behavioral change, because they may reward selfish behavior (Thøgersen 2003). Indeed, according to Bolderdijk et al. (2013), appealing only to economic self-interest is not always effective in securing behavior change. While PV investments increased tremendously during the government subsidy in South Tyrol, they stopped after the government subsidy ended in 2015 (Braito et al. 2017). Thus generous financial incentives do “… rarely lead to sustainable behavior change” (Axon et al. 2018, p. 583). Moreover, the PV policy in South Tyrol missed the opportunity to promote positive spillover effects by accompanying financial subsidies with a communication strategy.
Lanzini and Thøgersen (2014) attest that spillover effects of monetary support are at least as strong as those of verbal encouragement and praise. According to Lacasse (2016), highlighting environmentally friendly behavior helps to strengthen environmental self-identity, which in turn supports positive spillovers. Thus, labeling PV investments and environmental behaviors could prevent negative and reinforce positive spillovers (Geng et al. 2019). This is evident in the study case in Styria. It shows that more substantial positive spillovers arise when monetary incentives are complemented by additional instruments that appeal to intrinsic values. In particular, the case of CSPP shows that knowledge and communication, along with engagement, are potential factors that can change habitual energy behavior (Pothitou et al. 2016). For sustainability initiatives, the idea of behavioral spillover effects is attractive because such effects can lead to holistic behavioral changes in a cost-effective manner. Furthermore, it is tantamount to a missed opportunity if policies promote negative spillover effects, such as rebound effects.

Acknowledgements:

The research leading to this article was conducted within the Doctoral School of Sustainable Development (dokNE) at the University of Natural Resources and Life Sciences, Vienna (BOKU), jointly funded by BOKU, the Austrian Federal Ministry of Science, Research and Economy, the federal provinces of Vienna and Lower Austria, and BILLA.
Open Access Dieses Kapitel wird unter der Creative Commons Namensnennung 4.0 International Lizenz (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​deed.​de) veröffentlicht, welche die Nutzung, Vervielfältigung, Bearbeitung, Verbreitung und Wiedergabe in jeglichem Medium und Format erlaubt, sofern Sie den/die ursprünglichen Autor(en) und die Quelle ordnungsgemäß nennen, einen Link zur Creative Commons Lizenz beifügen und angeben, ob Änderungen vorgenommen wurden.
Die in diesem Kapitel enthaltenen Bilder und sonstiges Drittmaterial unterliegen ebenfalls der genannten Creative Commons Lizenz, sofern sich aus der Abbildungslegende nichts anderes ergibt. Sofern das betreffende Material nicht unter der genannten Creative Commons Lizenz steht und die betreffende Handlung nicht nach gesetzlichen Vorschriften erlaubt ist, ist für die oben aufgeführten Weiterverwendungen des Materials die Einwilligung des jeweiligen Rechteinhabers einzuholen.
Fußnoten
1
A spillover describes the phenomenon that changing one behavior (B1) leads to other behavioral changes (B2). According to Lanzini and Thøgersen (2014, p. 381), it "… implies that acting in a pro-environmental way changes (i.e., increases or decreases) a person's likelihood or extent of performing another/other pro-environmental behavior(s)". Spillovers (B2) can be triggered by the original behavior (B1) but also by the trigger of B1, which can be external (e.g., intervention) or internal (e.g., a person's values, motives, identity) (Maki et al. 2019). However, spillovers can also result from a change in a person's attitude after performing B1 (Henn et al. 2020).
 
2
The province of South Tyrol is located in the middle of the Alps and belonged to Austria until 1920. Almost 70 % of the population still speaks an Austrian dialect. South Tyrol has similar socio-economic and sociocultural conditions and mentalities as Austria, but a different political context.
 
3
A quasi-field experiment is an alternative to a field experiment, that would require study participants to be examined before and after their PV investment, which is not feasible.
 
4
Private-sphere behavior has a direct impact on the environment, is associated with tangible, everyday practices (Doyle 2013), and can take many forms, such as engaging in environmental movements, green shopping or voting, recycling, or adopting other forms of energy use or mobility.
 
5
Three questionnaires were excluded from the outset because they were half empty or had a deviating response pattern.
 
6
The assumption of an approximate normal distribution for each group was tested using the Shapiro–Wilk test, the Skew-Kurtosis test, and a graphical examination.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat BMK (Bundesministerium für Klimaschutz, Umwelt, Energie, Mobilität, Innovation und Technologie) (2013). Der österreichische Fußabdruck-Rechner. Wien, Österreich. Abgerufen am 18. November 2013 von http://www.mein-fussabdruck.at/. BMK (Bundesministerium für Klimaschutz, Umwelt, Energie, Mobilität, Innovation und Technologie) (2013). Der österreichische Fußabdruck-Rechner. Wien, Österreich. Abgerufen am 18. November 2013 von http://​www.​mein-fussabdruck.​at/​.
Zurück zum Zitat Field, A., Miles, J., & Field, Z. (2012). Discovering Statistics Using R. London, UK & Thousand Oaks, CA, US: Sage. Field, A., Miles, J., & Field, Z. (2012). Discovering Statistics Using R. London, UK & Thousand Oaks, CA, US: Sage.
Zurück zum Zitat Moberg, K. R., Aall, C., Dorner, F., Reimerson, E., Ceron, J.-P., Sköld, B., Sovacool, B. K., & Piana, V. (2019). Mobility, food and housing: Responsibility, individual consumption and demand-side policies in European deep decarbonisation pathways. Energy Efficiency, 12, 497–519. https://doi.org/10.1007/S12053-018-9708-7.CrossRef Moberg, K. R., Aall, C., Dorner, F., Reimerson, E., Ceron, J.-P., Sköld, B., Sovacool, B. K., & Piana, V. (2019). Mobility, food and housing: Responsibility, individual consumption and demand-side policies in European deep decarbonisation pathways. Energy Efficiency, 12, 497–519. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​S12053-018-9708-7.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nash, N., Whitmarsh, L., Capstick, S., Hargreaves, T., Poortinga, W., Thomas, G., Sautkina, E., & Xenias, D. (2017). Climate‐relevant behavioral spillover and the potential contribution of social practice theory. WIREs Climate Change, 8, e481. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcc.481.CrossRef Nash, N., Whitmarsh, L., Capstick, S., Hargreaves, T., Poortinga, W., Thomas, G., Sautkina, E., & Xenias, D. (2017). Climate‐relevant behavioral spillover and the potential contribution of social practice theory. WIREs Climate Change, 8, e481. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​wcc.​481.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nash, N., Whitmarsh, L., Capstick, S., Thøgerson, J., Gouveia, V., de Carvalho, R., Araújo, R., Harder, M. K., Wang, X., & Liu, Y. (2019). Reflecting on behavioral spillover in context: How do behavioral motivations and awareness catalyze other environmentally responsible actions in Brazil, China, and Denmark? Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 788. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00788. Nash, N., Whitmarsh, L., Capstick, S., Thøgerson, J., Gouveia, V., de Carvalho, R., Araújo, R., Harder, M. K., Wang, X., & Liu, Y. (2019). Reflecting on behavioral spillover in context: How do behavioral motivations and awareness catalyze other environmentally responsible actions in Brazil, China, and Denmark? Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 788. https://​doi.​org/​10.​3389/​fpsyg.​2019.​00788.
Zurück zum Zitat Steg, L., Perlaviciute, G., Sovacool, B. K., Bonaiuto, M., Diekmann, A., Filippini, M., Hinddriks, F., Jacobbson Bergstad, C., Matthies, E., Matti, S., Mulder, M., Nilsson, A., Pahl, S., Roggenkamp, M., Schuitema, G., Stern, P. C., Tavoni, M., Thøgerson, J., & Woerderman, E. (2021). A research agenda to better understand the human dimensions of energy transitions. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 672776. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.672776.CrossRef Steg, L., Perlaviciute, G., Sovacool, B. K., Bonaiuto, M., Diekmann, A., Filippini, M., Hinddriks, F., Jacobbson Bergstad, C., Matthies, E., Matti, S., Mulder, M., Nilsson, A., Pahl, S., Roggenkamp, M., Schuitema, G., Stern, P. C., Tavoni, M., Thøgerson, J., & Woerderman, E. (2021). A research agenda to better understand the human dimensions of energy transitions. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 672776. https://​doi.​org/​10.​3389/​fpsyg.​2021.​672776.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Energy Prosumers’ Spillovers and the Policy Effect: Comparing Two Alpine Valleys in Styria and South Tyrol
verfasst von
Michael Braito
Copyright-Jahr
2022
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-36562-2_8

Premium Partner